'People will forgive you for being wrong, but they will never forgive you for being right - especially if events prove you right while proving them wrong.' Thomas Sowell
Search This Blog
Showing posts with label lose. Show all posts
Showing posts with label lose. Show all posts
Thursday, 18 March 2021
Tuesday, 19 April 2016
These are the psychological tricks both sides of the EU debate are playing on you - and how to recognise them
What sounds worse: a shortfall of 6 per cent of GDP resulting from Brexit, or a loss of £4,300 per household?
Ben Chu in The Independent
Imagine you’re lucky. Imagine you receive £50 from a benefactor. But, oh dear, there’s a problem with the gift. It turns out too much was paid out. There has to be a financial correction. So you’re faced with a choice.
So would you rather keep £20? Or lose £30? Think very quickly. Did you initially lean towards keeping £20? Many people do. But of course they amount to the same thing. You’d still have £20 whichever option you picked.
So what’s going on? Why did £20 look more appealing? That’s the brain’s “system one” at work, according to psychologists. Studies show that the reactive human mind sees the “keep” flashing in red lights before there’s any mental arithmetic (even before trivial calculations such as subtracting £30 from £50). And the word “loss” is also deeply off-putting to the mind’s system one. A quick decision framed as a straight choice between “keep” and “lose” will usually see “lose” rejected.
The mental arithmetic is “system two” and it takes much longer to be activated in most of us than system one. Sadly, many of us don’t even bother activating system two before making decisions at all.
Advertisers are aware of this bias. That’s why they often frame propositions in terms of how much money people can keep rather than how much they’ve lost in the past. “Keep more of your savings income by opening an ISA”, “Keep more of your money when you shop with us”, and so on.
Political advertisers are on to it too. That’s why the Leave campaign ahead of June’s European Union referendum have been emphasising so heavily the prize of keeping the UK’s £13bn annual contribution to the EU Budget. They emphasise what we can keep by voting to leave. Yet the Remain campaign is familiar with this tactic too. That’s why they emphasise the 3m UK jobs “linked to trade with the rest of Europe”. We naturally want to keep all those jobs, don’t we?
Both claims are actually tendentious. The £13bn is the gross contribution of the UK to Brussels – it doesn’t account for the money the UK receives back. And it’s silly to imply that 3m jobs would disappear overnight in the event of a Brexit. That would only happen if all trade between Britain and the Continent came to a sudden halt – something no one seriously expects. But the campaigners are not really trying to impart useful information with their soundbites – they’re aiming at the system one part of your brain.
That’s by no means the only psychological bias battleground in this referendum campaign. Psychologists talk of the power of “framing”. Which sounds more appealing: 90 per cent fat-free or 10 per cent fat? Advertisers know the answer, which is why one never sees the latter formulation even though they describe the same product.
Now consider which sounds like a more compelling argument in the context of an EU membership vote. “Almost half of everything we sell to the rest of the world we sell to Europe,” says the Stronger in Europe campaign. “British reliance on trade with the EU has fallen to an all-time low,” proclaim the Outers. The fact that both sound compelling - and both describe the same statistics - shows that the two campaigns grasp the importance of framing.
There’s more. What sounds worse: a shortfall of 6 per cent of GDP resulting from Brexit, or a loss of £4,300 per household? For many people it will be the latter figure, heavily highlighted by George Osborne yesterday. But, again, they amount to the same thing. £4,300 is merely the 6 per cent of GDP translated into cash terms and divided by all the 26m households in the country.
So why does £4,300 sound more off-putting to most people? Here we have the “ratio bias” at work. In any ratio there is the numerator and the denominator. In the two statistics above “6” and “£4,400” are the numerators. And “GDP” and “per household” are the denominators. Studies show that the system one part of our brain is more sensitive to big numbers in the numerator of ratios, and often neglects the denominators. So £4,300 sets off larger movements in many brains because, quite simply, it’s a bigger sounding figure than 6.
Consider another example. Which is the more compelling fact: “200,000 UK businesses trade with the EU” or: “Only 6 per cent of UK firms export to the EU”? The first is from the Stronger in Europe website. The second is from Vote Leave. Here the Outers are trying to use the ratio bias to minimise the sense of importance of the EU as a trading partner for British firms - and the Inners are doing precisely the opposite.
We are profoundly influenced by the framing of statistics. Quite understandably, politicians and campaigners seek to manipulate your system one brain. “I just feel I don’t know who to trust and I need a voice I can trust,” said a member of a panel of “undecided” referendum voters on the BBC’s Newsnight last night. But that benign and trustworthy figure does not exist. The way the facts are laid out will depend on the way the person wants the facts to be framed. Asking for someone to do the job for you - and placing your trust in them - essentially means asking that person to steer you in one way or the other.
If people genuinely want to make up their minds without bias, they are on their own. And their only trustworthy guide is their own brain’s system two.
Ben Chu in The Independent
Imagine you’re lucky. Imagine you receive £50 from a benefactor. But, oh dear, there’s a problem with the gift. It turns out too much was paid out. There has to be a financial correction. So you’re faced with a choice.
So would you rather keep £20? Or lose £30? Think very quickly. Did you initially lean towards keeping £20? Many people do. But of course they amount to the same thing. You’d still have £20 whichever option you picked.
So what’s going on? Why did £20 look more appealing? That’s the brain’s “system one” at work, according to psychologists. Studies show that the reactive human mind sees the “keep” flashing in red lights before there’s any mental arithmetic (even before trivial calculations such as subtracting £30 from £50). And the word “loss” is also deeply off-putting to the mind’s system one. A quick decision framed as a straight choice between “keep” and “lose” will usually see “lose” rejected.
The mental arithmetic is “system two” and it takes much longer to be activated in most of us than system one. Sadly, many of us don’t even bother activating system two before making decisions at all.
Advertisers are aware of this bias. That’s why they often frame propositions in terms of how much money people can keep rather than how much they’ve lost in the past. “Keep more of your savings income by opening an ISA”, “Keep more of your money when you shop with us”, and so on.
Political advertisers are on to it too. That’s why the Leave campaign ahead of June’s European Union referendum have been emphasising so heavily the prize of keeping the UK’s £13bn annual contribution to the EU Budget. They emphasise what we can keep by voting to leave. Yet the Remain campaign is familiar with this tactic too. That’s why they emphasise the 3m UK jobs “linked to trade with the rest of Europe”. We naturally want to keep all those jobs, don’t we?
Both claims are actually tendentious. The £13bn is the gross contribution of the UK to Brussels – it doesn’t account for the money the UK receives back. And it’s silly to imply that 3m jobs would disappear overnight in the event of a Brexit. That would only happen if all trade between Britain and the Continent came to a sudden halt – something no one seriously expects. But the campaigners are not really trying to impart useful information with their soundbites – they’re aiming at the system one part of your brain.
That’s by no means the only psychological bias battleground in this referendum campaign. Psychologists talk of the power of “framing”. Which sounds more appealing: 90 per cent fat-free or 10 per cent fat? Advertisers know the answer, which is why one never sees the latter formulation even though they describe the same product.
Now consider which sounds like a more compelling argument in the context of an EU membership vote. “Almost half of everything we sell to the rest of the world we sell to Europe,” says the Stronger in Europe campaign. “British reliance on trade with the EU has fallen to an all-time low,” proclaim the Outers. The fact that both sound compelling - and both describe the same statistics - shows that the two campaigns grasp the importance of framing.
There’s more. What sounds worse: a shortfall of 6 per cent of GDP resulting from Brexit, or a loss of £4,300 per household? For many people it will be the latter figure, heavily highlighted by George Osborne yesterday. But, again, they amount to the same thing. £4,300 is merely the 6 per cent of GDP translated into cash terms and divided by all the 26m households in the country.
So why does £4,300 sound more off-putting to most people? Here we have the “ratio bias” at work. In any ratio there is the numerator and the denominator. In the two statistics above “6” and “£4,400” are the numerators. And “GDP” and “per household” are the denominators. Studies show that the system one part of our brain is more sensitive to big numbers in the numerator of ratios, and often neglects the denominators. So £4,300 sets off larger movements in many brains because, quite simply, it’s a bigger sounding figure than 6.
Consider another example. Which is the more compelling fact: “200,000 UK businesses trade with the EU” or: “Only 6 per cent of UK firms export to the EU”? The first is from the Stronger in Europe website. The second is from Vote Leave. Here the Outers are trying to use the ratio bias to minimise the sense of importance of the EU as a trading partner for British firms - and the Inners are doing precisely the opposite.
We are profoundly influenced by the framing of statistics. Quite understandably, politicians and campaigners seek to manipulate your system one brain. “I just feel I don’t know who to trust and I need a voice I can trust,” said a member of a panel of “undecided” referendum voters on the BBC’s Newsnight last night. But that benign and trustworthy figure does not exist. The way the facts are laid out will depend on the way the person wants the facts to be framed. Asking for someone to do the job for you - and placing your trust in them - essentially means asking that person to steer you in one way or the other.
If people genuinely want to make up their minds without bias, they are on their own. And their only trustworthy guide is their own brain’s system two.
Sunday, 22 April 2012
Aggressive Captaincy
|
|||
Related Links
Review 2011 : 'We need a deep point. What if he gets four?'
Players/Officials:
Michael Clarke
Series/Tournaments:
Australia tour of West Indies
Teams:
Australia
|
Captains: follow Clarke's lead
In the long run, captains who aim to win and set fields that will get them wickets are the ones who will succeed and last
April 22, 2012
Michael Clarke
is quickly establishing a well-deserved reputation for brave and
aggressive captaincy. His entertaining approach is based on one premise:
trying to win the match from the opening delivery. This should be the
aim of all international captains, but sadly it isn't.
In every era there are Test captains who prefer to attain a position of
safety before they go all out for victory. These captains are frightened
stiff of the Michael Clarkes, who make it obvious they are not
interested in a draw. At least 50% of international captains consider a
draw to be a good result and when that option is removed they easily
panic.
The first thing a captain like Clarke understands is that he will lose
some matches in constantly striving for victory. Once that premise is
accepted the captain has reached the stage where he hates to lose but
doesn't fear it. There's a huge difference: the latter is a positive
state where the captain will do everything in his power to win; the
former a mindset where the captain sets out not to lose.
An important indicator of a captain's thinking is his field placings. A
positive captain will always make the opposing batsmen feel their very
existence is threatened. Through his field placings he allows his
bowlers to turn at the top of their mark and see where a wicket (other
than bowled, lbw or through the batsman's stupidity) can be claimed.
A bowler operating to a purely containing field is like Zorro without
his sword; he's not very threatening. There has been plenty of
discussion on whether the shorter forms of the game will adversely
affect batting techniques and turn bowlers into cannon fodder. What the
50- and 20-over matches have actually had a marked influence on is field
placings.
Whereas the No. 1 priority, by a wide margin, used to be taking wickets,
followed by saving singles, and then, way off in the distance, stopping
boundaries, in the mind of the modern captain the last one has assumed
far too much importance.
The almost robotic use in Test matches of a deep cover point and a
backward square leg on the boundary, regardless of whether the ball is
being played in that direction, borders on mindless captaincy. When a
fieldsman is unemployed for half an hour but the captain still retains
him in that position, you have to wonder: who appointed this captain?
The change in attitude to field placings is perfectly summed up with some typical Caribbean humour and common sense.
Former West Indies fast bowler Herman Griffith was captaining a Barbados
club side in the 1930s once, and called on his debutant offspinner to
have a trundle. "Where do you want the field?" asked Griffith politely.
"I'd like a deep-backward square leg, a midwicket on the boundary and a long-on and long-off," replied the youngster.
"Give me the ball," growled Griffith.
Not unreasonably, the young man asked, "Why?"
"You intending to bowl shite," came the forthright answer.
Nowadays, most bowlers would be horrified if the captain didn't
automatically give him a number of protective fielders in the deep.
Clarke is not such a captain.
Sadly, his latest gambit - a challenging declaration at the Queen's Park Oval,
which was answered with equal bravado by Darren Sammy, failed because
of inclement weather. Nevertheless, it's to be hoped their positive
endeavours have acted as a sharp reminder to the administrators.
In Test cricket the captain is allowed free rein. We've seen in the case
of Clarke and Sammy what's possible when two captains use a bit of
imagination and have a desire to produce a result. It's impossible to
legislate for captaincy imagination. In the 50-over game, which is
highly regulated through a variety of Powerplays, and bowling and field
restrictions, there is less real captaincy involved.
Wherever possible, the captaincy should be left to the skippers, and
those with imagination will prosper. Hopefully those who lack
inspiration and the nerve to face a challenge will be quickly replaced
by the selectors.
Friday, 25 November 2011
Is there room for intellectuals in cricket?
Ed Smith in Cricinfo 24/11/2011
WG Grace thought reading books was bad for your batting. "You'll never
catch me that way," he scoffed. The story serves as a metaphor for
sport's suspicion of intellectual life. Thinkers, readers, curious
minds: do we really want them clogging up the supposedly optimistic,
forward-looking atmosphere of a cricket team?
Cricket is still grappling with the terrible news that Peter Roebuck -
one of sport's genuine intellectuals - jumped to his death from his
hotel balcony as he was being questioned by South African police about a
sexual assault charge. The circumstances of Roebuck's death were
clearly atypical. Nonetheless, his life - especially those parts of his
life that belonged to cricket - fit the pattern of an intellectual who
never quite settled into an easy relationship with the sport he loved.
Other sports are arguably even more anti-intellectual than cricket.
Football never entirely understood Pat Nevin. Graeme Le Saux was
subjected to homophobic chants and abuse. He wasn't gay, of course - his
"sin" was to read serious newspapers such as the Guardian.
In Ball Four, the New York Yankees pitcher Jim Bouton's wrote the
first great exposé of major league sport. He described how the
management encouraged, almost forced, their players to drink beer after
matches. That Bouton preferred milk was thought to be proof that he
wasn't a real bloke. He was made to feel guilty for being intellectually
curious. Bouton wrote admiringly about one soulmate who liked to lie
down in open fields and read poetry. But his intellectual team-mate
subsequently denied it.
Let's not pretend that there aren't tensions between thinking and
competing. I turned professional at probably my most openly intellectual
phase, when I had just graduated from Cambridge University. Perhaps too
many things had all happened too soon for me - I was only 20 when I
graduated. And we were young and callow and could be a pretentious
bunch, with the intellectual bar set ludicrously high. We thought
nothing of being habitually dismissive - forgive us, but being
dismissive was the style.
From that rarefied academic environment, dominated by abstract thinking
and academic competitiveness, I stepped straight into a first-class
cricket dressing room. It was a massive change and gave me a huge jolt.
And I'm sure I didn't always handle it well. On one away trip, my
room-mate picked up the book on my bedside table. It was Experience and Its Modes, a densely argued book by the philosopher Michael Oakeshott. I'll never forget the expression on his face.
Mike Atherton and I once discussed whether intellectuals had any place
in modern sport. The best defence is that good sports teams embrace
diversity. They are open to all different types, including players who
do not naturally fit the stereotype of a team player. The best teams are
liberal in the deepest sense. They do not stifle independent thinkers
or left-field ideas. They do not enforce conventional, middle-brow
behaviour.
For that reason, the worst combination for a sporting intellectual is a
losing team and a weak, insecure captain. A losing team searches for
scapegoats. During times of insecurity and pressure, as history shows,
human groups often turn on unconventional individuals. Insecure leaders
want to be surrounded by players of limited intelligence. It is easier
that way.
Surprisingly, however, the team's "intellectual" usually has little to
fear from the anti-intellectual jocks. No, the real threat comes from
the jealousy of the nearly man, the player who fancies himself as a
thinker and resents the competition. Team splits often begin with the
manipulations of jealous, thwarted players who think they are cleverer
than they are.
The best teams are liberal in the deepest sense. They do not stifle independent thinkers or left-field ideas | |||
Winning, of course, always helps. A winning team is more inclined to
look for the good in unusual players. Looking back on my career, the
happiest times were when I played under secure captains and coaches. My
father, a lifelong teacher, often told me that weak headmasters appoint
unthreatening deputies, but strong headmasters back themselves to handle
more restless and independent people. I suspect that was one of Adam
Hollioake's great strengths as a captain: he encouraged people to be
themselves. He could do that because he was happy in his own skin. "I
enjoy my life, I want my team-mates to enjoy theirs" - that was always
the impression I got from Adam.
Roebuck, I sense, craved that kind of acceptance - in cricket and in
life. He once emailed me a long, uncorrected series of acute perceptions
and observations. It was classic Roebuck - staccato, direct and
unsparing, especially of himself. He wrote: "I realised that I had not
actually enjoyed cricket at all. Englishmen love to suffer! I played one
creative innings at Somerset and that's the only press cutting I kept. I
never really dared again."
He was determined to avoid those errors in his career as a writer.
"Always tell the truth in your own way. As a journalist I never go into
the office, as I say nothing happens in offices! One has to work hard
not to get sucked into 'the operation'. But dare one tread that path? Do
you? Professionalism is not an enemy but it has become a mantra. I
concentrate entirely in staying fresh - or else work becomes tired,
cynical, useless. Cleverness is an easy substitute for thought. Begin
afresh afresh as Larkin wrote."
That "Do you?" was one of the most direct challenges I have had put to me.
He had so much more thinking to do, so many more insights to develop.
Instead, his innings did not run its full and proper course. "A player
goes through three stages - natural, complicated, simple - not many
reach that last stage but the journey cannot be avoided. Failure is the
problem," he wrote to me.
Roebuck's three-stage journey applies to life as well as to batting. It
is deeply sad that Roebuck's life ended while it was still very much at
the complicated stage. One day, I hope, the intellectual will find it
easier to find a natural role in professional sport.
Former England, Kent and Middlesex batsman Ed Smith is a writer with the Times.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)